Logo Medical Science Monitor

Call: +1.631.470.9640
Mon - Fri 10:00 am - 02:00 pm EST

Contact Us

Logo Medical Science Monitor Logo Medical Science Monitor Logo Medical Science Monitor

29 March 2022: Clinical Research  

Cognitive and Socioemotional Development at 5 and 9 Years of Age of Children Born with Very Low Birth Weight and Extremely Low Birth Weight in the Czech Republic

Lucie Švandová ORCID logo1BDEF, Radek Ptáček1ABDG*, Martina Vňuková1BCDE, Hana Ptáčková1BF, Martin Anders1E, Petr Bob1E, Simon Weissenberger ORCID logo1E, Daniela Marková2AG, Ivan Sebalo ORCID logo34CD, Jiří Raboch1EG, Michal Goetz5ABDE

DOI: 10.12659/MSM.935784

Med Sci Monit 2022; 28:e935784

0 Comments

Abstract

BACKGROUND: There is a high prevalence of cognitive and socioemotional dysfunction in very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) and extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) children. This study from the Czech Republic aimed to compare the cognitive and socioemotional development at 5 and 9 years of age of children born with VLBW/ELBW with children born with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g).

MATERIAL AND METHODS: The clinical group consisted of 118 VLBW/ELBW children and the control group consisted of 101 children with NBW at ages 5 to 9 years. The research battery included selected subscales from the Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS), A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment – second edition (NEPSY-II), and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). Data were analyzed using STATA IC v. 15 software and G*Power (descriptive statistic, analysis of variance (ANOVA), correlations, multivariate analysis of variance – MANOVA, post hoc power analysis).

RESULTS: We found a statistically significant difference in cognitive and socioemotional development between children with VLBW/ELBW and those with NBW. The average intelligence quotient (IQ) of VLBW/ELBW children was 96.38, while that of NBW children was 12.98 points higher (P<0.001). NBW children achieved better results on all subtests of the IDS (P<0.001) as well as in affect recognition (P<0.001). All results for both groups were within normal range. Parents of VLBW/ELBW children did not recognize impaired executive functioning (P=0.494).

CONCLUSIONS: This study has shown significant cognitive and socioemotional deficit in children born with VLBW and ELBW when evaluated at 5 and 9 years of age.

Keywords: Child Development, cognitive dysfunction, Infant, Extremely Low Birth Weight, Infant, Very Low Birth Weight, Analysis of Variance, Birth Weight, Child, Child, Preschool, Cognition, Czech Republic, Humans, Infant, Newborn

Background

All citizens, residents, and employees in the Czech Republic have access to healthcare via compulsory general healthcare insurance [1]. However, current medical and psychological care provided for premature children and their families remains inadequate, as it is largely unsystematic and is directly dependent on the varying capacities of the local perinatal centers [2]. Furthermore, continuous monitoring of mental development is mandated only from infancy through 2 years of age [2]. This is not sufficient for the care of low-birth-weight children and children, as it is often impossible to generate an accurate prognosis of their developmental trajectory at this young age [2,3]. Birth weight is the first weight of the fetus or neonate obtained after birth. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), birth weight is divided into low birth weight (LBW <2500 g), very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g), and extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) [4]. Premature birth before the 37th week of pregnancy and/or growth faltering in the womb increase the risk of low birth weight [5]. The number of LBW children born in the Czech Republic has been increasing in recent years. In 2018, 6.9% of all live children born in the Czech Republic were identified as LBW [6]. Along with efforts to reduce child mortality, experts have also focused on treatment options that might be used to reduce morbidity both in the neonatal period and later on in life [2]. Children with ELBW are the group that is most at risk for these complications [7–9].

There is strong evidence indicating that children born with VLBW and ELBW have a greater predisposition to develop cognitive deficits and/or delayed cognitive development [11,13–18]. Impaired attention, memory, perceptual-motor skills, and visual-motor skills are among the deficits that are most frequently reported in studies of cognitive development of VLBW/ELBW children [9,11,18–20]. Speech and language disabilities or delays (both receptive and expressive) have also been reported [12,21–25]. Impairment has also been reported in the areas that include executive functions [7,26–28]. The aforementioned deficits are detected most often at school age, when children are facing increasing demands, notably in mathematics, reading, and spelling [25,29–34]. Although studies that address measurements of intelligence quotient (IQ) in VLBW and ELBW children show great variability [35–37], it is clear that VLBW/ELBW children may have a limited capacity for intellectual and overall cognitive development compared to children born at term at the normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) [37,38]. Some of the deficits observed among VLBW/ELBW children relate to the development of socioemotional competencies [7,8,39–42]. Johnson and Marlow described the “preterm behavioral phenotype”, which is characterized by an increased risk of developing symptoms and disorders associated with inattention/hyperactivity, emotional, and social difficulties. Individuals with this phenotype are also at an increased risk for internalizing rather than externalizing problems [43]. Children born with VLBW or ELBW are also more vulnerable to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and impulsivity, with symptoms more likely to persist into adulthood [39,44,45]. Other studies report an increased risk of pervasive developmental disorders; the incidence of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) has been reported to range from 3.65% to 8% among VLBW/ELBW children [44,46]. These children also experience a higher incidence of depressive symptoms, low self-esteem, and anxiety disorders [8,47]. However, a whole population longitudinal study from birth to adulthood found that VLBW children were not at persistently increased risk for anxiety and mood disorders [48]. Other studies have reported difficulties in affect recognition among VLBW/ELBW children at preschool age [42,49]; some of these deficits may persist into middle childhood, notably with those involving the recognition of anger [50,51]. Problems with affect recognition and facial expressions result in an overall impairment in social skills and social adaptation [50], and can also lead to problems with the regulation of emotions [52,53], with a significant impact on relationships with peers, parents, and teachers [54,55]. Difficulties with social adaptation tend to appear early and persist into adolescence [56]. Although a number of the above studies demonstrate that these impairments can have a major long-term impact on children and their families and require long-term support and interventions, in the Czech Republic these children are not given the long-term attention they need [2].

To the best of our knowledge, there are no published studies that explore the relationship between birth weight and cognitive and socioemotional development at 5 and 9 years of age of children residing in the Czech Republic. The development of these children has not yet been mapped. In 2015/2016, we conducted a research project entitled “New Methods in the Follow-Up Care for Children with Perinatal Stress” at the Center for Follow-up Care of Ex-preterm Babies within the Department of Pediatrics and Inherited Metabolic Disorders, First Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and General University Hospital in Prague. Most often, babies born in one of Prague’s maternity hospitals with a perinatal burden come under the care of the center. This was a large-scale project focused on the development of multidisciplinary care for children with specific perinatal burdens. Over the course of a given day, each child underwent a comprehensive evaluation that included pneumatological, routine pediatric, rehabilitative, psychological, and psychiatric assessments. One goal of the study was to evaluate the mental development of VLBW/ELBW children who were then between 5 and 9 years of age. Socioemotional and cognitive development was measured by subtests selected from the Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) [57] and Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II) [58,59]. Executive functions were examined using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [60]. This project was followed by the present study, which aimed to compare the cognitive and socioemotional development at 5 and 9 years of age of 118 children born with VLBW/ELBW with 101 children born with NBW. Based on our review of the literature, we proposed 4 hypotheses. H1 0: There is no statistically significant difference between the cognitive development of children with VLBW/ELBW and children with normal birth weight. H1: Children with normal birth weight have statistically significantly better cognitive development than children with VLBW/ELBW. H2 0: There is no statistically significant difference in socioemotional development between children with VLBW/ELBW and children with normal birth weight. H2: Children with normal birth weight have statistically significantly better socioemotional development than children with VLBW/ELBW.

Material and Methods

ETHICS STATEMENT:

The research project has been carefully considered in all aspects of its research and ethical content by the institutional review board for “Medical Psychology and Psychopathology” at the First Faculty of Medicine, Charles University. The parents of the children in both groups were informed of the goals and the procedures involved in this research project, and all signed a written informed consent form. An emphasis was placed on the current well-being of the child throughout the evaluation period.

PROCEDURE AND PARTICIPANTS:

One hundred and eighteen children aged 5 to 9 years were selected for participation in this study. Inclusion criteria were: age 5 to 9 years who were VLBW or ELBW. All children were monitored and treated at the Center for Follow-up Care of Ex-preterm Babies. This was a non-probability sampling method as the children and parents were selected from among those who were already receiving care in this center. Children with severe sensory impairments, mutism, and severe ASD were excluded from the study. The research studies took place primarily during the morning hours at the Center for Follow-up Care of Ex-preterm Babies within the Department of Pediatrics and Inherited Metabolic Disorders, First Faculty of Medicine, Charles University and General University Hospital in Prague, Czech Republic. The duration of the assessment of the child was about 1 hour, plus an interview with the parent for about 15 minutes.

A control group recruited in 2017–2019, included NBW children who were 5 to 9 years of age who were not receiving mental health care. The participants were selected from a voluntary response sample from Prague and its close surroundings. The control group included 101 children who underwent the psychological examination only. The examination took place at the Institute of Psychology, Prague, Czech Republic.

Children came for the assessment with their parent (mostly with their mother). Subsequently, the child and the parents were told what the assessment entailed. The child underwent the assessment without the presence of the parent. During testing, an emphasis was placed on the current well-being of the child. If the test situation was too stressful for the child or caused any distress or anxiety, the child was given some time to familiarize him/herself with the situation, take a break from the test, or go back to the parent. An alternative testing date could have been arranged, but in neither case was it necessary.

MEASURES:

IDS is appropriate for children at 5–10 years of age. The administration of the entire battery of tests typically requires 1.5–2 hours. For this reason, we administered selected subtests only, including those focused on visual perception, selective attention, phonological memory, visual-spatial memory, auditory memory, visual-motor skills, and receptive and expressive language, using validated Czech language standards [57].

NEPSY-II is a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests designed to evaluate neurocognitive development in preschoolers, children, and adolescents (ie, 3–16 years of age) [58,59]. We used only the subtest designed to evaluate affect recognition.

BRIEF is a questionnaire for parents or teachers that allows them to assess executive function in children aged 5 to 18 years. The BRIEF has 3 main indices: (1) Metacognition Index (MI) assesses the ability to initiate, organize, plan, and remember the steps needed to achieve a specific goal; (2) Behavior Regulation Index (BRI) evaluates the ability to manage and control behavior and emotional responses; and (3) Global Executive Composite (GEC – a composite of the 2 aforementioned indices) [60].

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS:

Data were analyzed using STATA IC v. 15 software. Post hoc statistical power was calculated in G*Power. Birth weight was coded as a discrete dichotomous variable (0 – control, 1 – VLBW/ELBW) and was used as the predictor (independent) variable in all of our analyses. IQ, IDS subscales, as well as the NEPSY-II and BRIEF subscales were linear continuous variables that were included as outcome (dependent) variables.

The overall IQ score as determined by the IDS was used as the single-outcome variable, and analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with the control group was used as a reference. ANOVA is a statistical method used to compare means of multiple groups to find out if the differences between them are statistically significant.

However, because the total IQ score is calculated from IDS subscales, our second analysis included the simultaneous use of visual perception, selective attention, phonological memory, visual-spatial memory, auditory memory, visual-motor skills, and receptive and expressive language as dependent variables and birth weight as the independent variable. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed to accommodate the co-existence of multiple outcome variables. MANOVA is similar to ANOVA in that it is a mean comparison test with multiple dependent variables. Correlations were performed in conjunction with this process because the dependent variables evaluated in the MANOVA should be correlated with one another.

Socioemotional development was also evaluated using ANOVA. In this case, the birth weight group was the independent variable, and the results of the affect recognition subtest of NEPSY-II were the dependent variables.

Responses from parents were evaluated in a separate MANOVA in which the BRIEF BRI and MI subscales were included as the dependent variables and birth weight as the independent variable.

The effect size was calculated using the η2 value obtained from the ANOVAs together with a post hoc power analysis.

Results

DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS:

One hundred and eighteen LBW children were enrolled in our study, including 59 boys (50%) and 59 girls (50%). Forty-one (34.8%) of the participants were VLBW children and 77 (65.3%) were ELBW (65.3%) children. The mean age was 76.5 months (range, 60–115 months). The average birth weight was 918.07 g (range 405–1470 g). The control group of 101 children included 53 boys (52.5%) and 48 girls (47.5%) girls. The mean age was 81.2 months (range 60–107 months). The average birth weight was 3363.55 g (range 2600–4800 g). Quantitative analysis revealed no correlation between gender or age (preschool or young school-age) and the overall IQ. Likewise, our analysis revealed no significant differences between the groups of VLBW and ELBW children (t [71]=0.37, P=0.71). Therefore, we divided the participants into 2 groups according to birth weight (ie, NBW and VLBW/ELBW cohorts) for all subsequent statistical analyses.

ANALYSES OF IDS:

ANOVA revealed significant differences in cognitive development (F [1.217]=41.49, P<0.001). Overall, birth weight explained only ~16% of the variance in IQ. The average IQ of VLBW/ELBW children was 96.38, while the average IQ of NBW children was 12.98 points higher (Table 1). The individual scores are presented in Figure 1. A post hoc power analysis of the IQ data revealed a statistical power of 0.99 (Table 2). Before computing the MANOVA for cognitive development, we evaluated the results from the IDS subscales in a correlation matrix (Table 3). The results revealed a moderate correlation between the results from the IDS subscales (Table 4). The Wilks’ lambda value was significant (F [8.210]=7.73, P<0.001) as were all the IDS subscales. Furthermore, our findings revealed that NBW children as a group performed better than VLBW/ELBW children on all subscales evaluated. The strongest associations (as per the R2 value) were observed for subscales of visual perception, phonological memory, and selective attention. The partial η2 (=0.22) was used to calculate the effect size, ie, f2(V)=0.22/(1–0.22)=0.29. A post hoc power analysis of overall cognitive development revealed a high statistical power (0.99; Table 5). Collectively, our findings revealed that NBW children performed better than VLBW/ELBW children on all cognitive domains measured by the IDS scale when evaluated at 5–9 years of age.

ANALYSIS OF NEPSY-II:

ANOVA revealed significant differences in socioemotional development (F [1.217]=11.79, P<0.001). Birth weight explained ~4.7% of the total variance in affect recognition (Table 6). VLBW/ELBW children achieved an average score of 9 on this evaluation, while NBW children achieved significantly higher scores. The individual scores on this subtest are shown in Figure 2. A post hoc power analysis revealed a high statistical power for this evaluation (Table 7). Collectively, our results revealed that NBW children (5–9 years of age) performed significantly better on subtests of affect recognition than children born with VLBW/ELBW.

ANALYSIS OF BRIEF:

Finally, results from the BRI and MI BRIEF subscales were evaluated by MANOVA. The results from tests using these subscales were based on responses to questionnaires completed by the parents of the study participants. The differences observed in this case did not reach statistical significance (F [2.216]=0.71, P=0.494).

Discussion

The results of this study revealed that VLBW/ELBW children have diminished levels of cognitive and socioemotional development compared to their NBW peers at 5–9 years of age. The average intelligence quotient (IQ) of children with NBW was higher. NBW children achieved better results on all subtests of the IDS as well as in affect recognition. All the results for both groups were within normal range. Parents of VLBW/ELBW children or children with NBW did not recognize impaired executive functioning.

While the average IQ of NBW children in our study was 109.36, ELBW/VLBW children had an average score of 96.38, representing a 12.98-point difference. These findings are consistent with the results of a meta-analysis published by Gu et al that reported lower (albeit normal) IQs among VLBW/ELBW children compared to those born at NBW [37]. Recently published systematic reviews document individuals with LBW to have around 5–12 points lower IQ scores [10,37,61]. These authors also reported a gradient relationship between different levels of LBW and IQ score [37]; interestingly, our results did not confirm this finding. However, we recognize that only 5 of 7 subscales used to determine IQ were performed in the study. The scores of the omitted subtests were included as weighted averages. Therefore, these results should be interpreted with caution. Our results revealed that NBW children performed significantly better than their VLBW/ELBW counterparts on all IDS subtests evaluated. These findings are consistent with those previously reported in the medical literature and include differences in selective attention, perceptual-motor skills, visual motor skills [9,11,19,20], memory (phonology, auditory, visual-spatial) [21,45], and language development [12,21,22,24]. We also found that NBW children performed significantly better than VLBW/ELBW children on subtests of affect recognition. These results are also in agreement with those reported in previously published studies [42,49–51]. Interestingly, parents did not report impaired executive function on the BRIEF questionnaire. This implies that the parents believe that their children are all able to manage and control behavior and emotional responses and also can initiate, organize, plan, and remember the steps needed to achieve a specific goal. We recognize that this is a fully subjective evaluation. We cannot rule out the possibility of a bias based on social desirability. Several studies reported that VLBW/ELBW children have impairments in executive functions, predominantly those involving planning and organization, cognitive flexibility, working memory, attention, inhibition processes, verbal fluency, and behavioral and emotional control [7,26–28]. Overall, our results are in general agreement with findings reported in the literature [7,8,40,44]. Our quantitative analysis revealed significant differences in cognitive and socioemotional development when comparing the VLBW/ELBW group to the NBW controls. However, it is important to recognize that the results of all the psychological evaluations of children (including those in the VLBW/ELBW group) were within the normal range. In other words, while the test scores of the VLBW/ELBW children were statistically different from those of NBW children, the results as a whole were not below the standardized average.

The comparatively strong results achieved by the children in our VLBW/ELBW cohort may relate directly to the individualized, long-term, and comprehensive care provided by a multidisciplinary team at our hospital. We recognize that this level of comprehensive care does not reach all VLBW/ELBW children in the Czech Republic. The review by Spittle et al (2015) suggests that early developmental interventions improve cognitive outcomes up to preschool age. Little evidence was found of an effect on long-term cognitive outcomes (up to school age). Were included 25 randomized or quasi-randomized controlled trials of early developmental interventions for preterm children. Variability among these early developmental intervention programs limits the conclusions that can be drawn about their effectiveness [62].

Going forward, it is important to focus on several steps. In the Czech Republic there are currently significant shortcomings in follow-up psychological care both for the premature baby and the parents and family. The availability of high-quality psychological services might be expanded together with efforts to provide standardized methods for screening, assessing, and increasing the awareness of the need to monitor the behavior of these children and their families [2]. Further insight into the potential mechanisms associated with the socioemotional and cognitive development of LBW/preterm children could be provided by neuroscience, such as further study of changes in the structural and functional architecture of brain networks involved in emotion regulation and other parts of the brain. To better understand the whole issue, it is desirable to perform further research, not only in young children, but also in adolescents and adults born with low birth weight. The consequences of low birth weight are not yet sufficiently mapped for these periods.

This study had several limitations. As noted above, all VLBW/ELBW children enrolled in this study have undergone extensive long-term monitoring at the Center for Follow-up Care of Ex-preterm Babies. This level of care far exceeds the current standards in the Czech Republic. Also, we recognize that all our participants were active volunteers. Thus, the participants may have included only children with particularly motivated parents who also provided their children with superior attention and care. We also recognize that our study lacked socio-economic demographic data. Parental education, employment, and income, as well as the number of siblings in the family, have all been identified as predictors of cognitive development [63–66].

As noted above, there is no standard Czech language version of the NEPSY-II. We had only limited decision-making capacity in this regard, as the instruments to be used for this study were selected by an outside expert commission. Furthermore, the omission of the structural and conceptual thinking subscales of the IDS limited our overall analysis of cognition. Other limitations relate to the testing conditions. During the course of one day, the children underwent a comprehensive psychological and physical examination. Although we all agreed that the psychological examination would be performed first to avoid possible bias caused by fatigue, this plan was not followed in all cases.

Conclusions

This study has shown significant cognitive and socioemotional deficits in children born with VLBW and ELBW when evaluated at 5 and 9 years of age. Although the study had several limitations, our results confirm that LBW remains a major risk factor for delayed mental development. We hope that the results of the study will raise awareness of the effects of perinatal stress and its impact on mental development. Our results highlight the need to improve the availability of systematic follow-up focused on both medical and psychological care of VLBW/ELBW children even after their discharge from perinatal centers.

Tables

Table 1. Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) comparison of intelligence quotient (IQ) and birth weight.The mean (M) IQ score was 100 with a standard deviation (SD) of 15. ANOVA revealed significant differences (F [1.217]=41.49, P<0.001). Overall, birth weight explained only ~16% of the variance in IQ. The average IQ of children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) was 96.38, while the average IQ of children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) was 12.98 points higher.Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) comparison of intelligence quotient (IQ) and birth weight.The mean (M) IQ score was 100 with a standard deviation (SD) of 15. ANOVA revealed significant differences (F [1.217]=41.49, P<0.001). Overall, birth weight explained only ~16% of the variance in IQ. The average IQ of children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) was 96.38, while the average IQ of children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) was 12.98 points higher. Table 2. Post hoc power analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ).A post-hoc power analysis revealed a statistical power of 0.99.Post hoc power analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ).A post-hoc power analysis revealed a statistical power of 0.99. Table 3. Correlation matrix of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).The results revealed a moderate correlation between the results from the IDS subscales.Correlation matrix of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).The results revealed a moderate correlation between the results from the IDS subscales. Table 4. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) and birth weight.Number of observations (N)=219. The Wilks’ lambda value was significant (F [8.210]=7.73, P<0.001) as were all the IDS subscales. The strongest associations (as per the R2 value) were observed for subscales of visual perception, phonological memory, and selective attention. Children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) as a group perform better than children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) on all subscales evaluated.Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) and birth weight.Number of observations (N)=219. The Wilks’ lambda value was significant (F [8.210]=7.73, P<0.001) as were all the IDS subscales. The strongest associations (as per the R2 value) were observed for subscales of visual perception, phonological memory, and selective attention. Children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) as a group perform better than children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) on all subscales evaluated. Table 5. Post hoc power analysis of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).A post-hoc power analysis of overall cognitive development revealed a high statistical power (0.99).Post hoc power analysis of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).A post-hoc power analysis of overall cognitive development revealed a high statistical power (0.99). Table 6. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II) and birth weight.The ANOVA revealed significant differences in socioemotional development (F [1.217]=11.79, P<0.001). Birth weight explained ~4.7% of the total variance in affect recognition.Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II) and birth weight.The ANOVA revealed significant differences in socioemotional development (F [1.217]=11.79, P<0.001). Birth weight explained ~4.7% of the total variance in affect recognition. Table 7. Post hoc power analysis of Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II).A post hoc power analysis revealed a high statistical power for this evaluation (0.89).Post hoc power analysis of Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II).A post hoc power analysis revealed a high statistical power for this evaluation (0.89).

References

1. Alexa J, Rečka L, Votápková J, Czech Republic: Health system review: Health Syst Transit, 2015; 17(1); 1-165

2. Marková D, Weberová-Chvílová M, Raušová P, Péče o předčasně narozené dítě : Kdy začíná a kdy končí: Čes-slov Pediat, 2014; 69(1); 53-62 [in Czech]

3. Hack M, Taylor HG, Drotar D, Poor predictive validity of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development for cognitive function of extremely low birth weight children at school age: Pediatrics, 2005; 116(2); 333-41

4. World Health Organization, 2016, World Health Organization 2015 Available from: https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/246208

5. United Nations Children’s Fund World Health Organization: Low birthweight estimates: Levels and trends 2000–2015, 2019, Geneva, World Health Organization Available from: https://www.unicef.org/media/96976/file/UNICEF-WHO-Low-Birthweight-estimates-2000-2015.pdf

6. ÚZIS ČR: Narození a zemřelí do 1 roku 2018, 2019, Prague, ÚZIS ČR Available from[in Czech]https://www.uzis.cz/res/f/008279/narzem2018.pdf

7. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J, Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcome in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children: Pediatrics, 2009; 124; 717-28

8. Mathewson KJ, Chow CH, Dobson KG, Mental health of extremely low birth weight survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis: Psychol Bull, 2017; 143; 347-83

9. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: A meta-analysis: JAMA, 2002; 288; 728-37

10. Upadhyay RP, Naik G, Choudhary TS, Cognitive and motor outcomes in children born low birth weight: A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies from South Asia: BMC Pediatr, 2019; 19(1); 1-15

11. Orchinik LJ, Taylor HG, Espy KA, Cognitive outcomes for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in kindergarten: J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 2011; 17; 1067-79

12. Stolt S, Matomäki J, Lind A, The prevalence and predictive value of weak language skills in children with very low birth weight – a longitudinal study: Acta Paediatr, 2014; 103; 651-58

13. Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Stability of general cognition in children born extremely preterm as they grow older: Good or bad news?: Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Educ, 2018; 103(4); F299-300

14. Stålnacke J, Lundequist A, Bohm B, Individual cognitive patterns and developmental trajectories after preterm birth: Child Neuropsychol, 2015; 21(5); 648-67

15. Mangin KS, Horwood LJ, Woodward LJ, Cognitive development trajectories of very preterm and typically developing children: Child Dev, 2017; 88(1); 282-98

16. Linsell L, Johnson S, Wolke D, Cognitive trajectories from infancy to early adulthood following birth before 26 weeks of gestation: A prospective, population-based cohort study: Arch Dis Child, 2018; 103(4); 363-70

17. Wong HS, Santhakumaran S, Cowan FM, Modi NMedicines for Neonates Investigator Group, Developmental assessments in preterm children: A meta-analysis: Pediatrics, 2016; 138(2); e20160251

18. Anderson PJ, Neuropsychological outcomes of children born very preterm: Semin Fetal Neonatal Med, 2014; 19(2); 90-96

19. Kulseng S, Jennekens-Schinkel A, Naess P, Very-low-birthweight and term small-for-gestational-age adolescents: Attention revisited: Acta Paediatr, 2006; 95; 224-30

20. Gidley Larson G, Baron IS, Neuromotor outcomes at school age after extremely low birth weight: Early detection of subtle signs: Neuropsychol, 2011; 25; 66-75

21. Debata P, Kumar J, Mukhopadhyay K, Screening for language delay between 6 months and 3 years of corrected age in very low birth weight children: Indian Pediatr, 2019; 56(6); 481-84

22. Barre N, Morgan A, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birth weight: A meta-analysis: J Pediatr, 2011; 158; 766-74

23. Guarini A, Marini A, Savini S, Linguistics features in children born very preterm at preschool age: Dev Med Child Neurol, 2016; 58(9); 949-56

24. Ortiz-Mantilla S, Choudhury N, Leevers H, Benasich AA, Understanding language and cognitive deficits in very low birth weight children: Dev Psychobiol, 2008; 50(2); 107-26

25. Vohr BR, Normal and Abnormal Neurodevelopmental and Behavioral Outcomes of Very Low-Birth Weight (VLBW) Infants: Neonatology: A Practical Approach to Neonatal Diseases, 2016; 1-19, Springer

26. Ritter BC, Perrig W, Steinlin M, Everts R, Cognitive and behavioral aspects of executive functions in children born very preterm: Child Neuropsychol, 2014; 20(2); 129-44

27. Luu TM, Ment L, Allan W, Executive and memory function in adolescents born very preterm: Pediatrics, 2011; 127; e639-46

28. van Houdt CA, Oosterlaan J, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis AG, Executive function deficits in children born preterm or at low birthweight: A meta-analysis: Dev Med Child Neurol, 2019; 61(9); 1015-24

29. Leijon I, Ingemansson F, Nelson N, Reading deficits in very low birthweight children are associated with vocabulary and attention issues at the age of seven: Acta Paediatr, 2016; 105(1); 60-68

30. Feder KP, Majnemer A, Bourbonnais D, Handwriting performance in preterm children compared with term peers at age 6 to 7 years: Dev Med Child Neurol, 2005; 47(3); 163-70

31. Allotey J, Zamora J, Cheong-See F, Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: A meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64061 children: BJOG, 2017; 125(1); 16-25

32. Takeuchi A, Ogino T, Koeda T, Intelligence test at preschool-age predicts reading difficulty among school-aged very low birth weight infants in Japan: Brain Dev, 2018; 40(9); 735-42

33. Akshoomoff N, Joseph RM, Taylor HG, Academic achievement deficits and their neuropsychological correlates in children born extremely preterm: J Dev Behav Pediatr, 2017; 38(8); 627-37

34. Johnson S, Strauss V, Gilmore C, Learning disabilities among extremely preterm children without neurosensory impairment: Comorbidity, neuropsychological profiles and scholastic outcomes: Early Hum Dev, 2016; 103; 69-75

35. Méio MD, Lopes CS, Morsch DS, Prognostic factors for cognitive development of very low birth weight premature children: Rev Saude Publica, 2003; 37(3); 311-18

36. Cooke RW, Foulder-Hughes L, Growth impairment in the very preterm and cognitive and motor performance at 7 years: Arch Dis Child, 2003; 88(6); 482-87

37. Gu H, Wang L, Liu L, A gradient relationship between low birth weight and IQ: A meta-analysis: Sci Rep, 2017; 7(1); 18035

38. Martinez-Cruz CF, Poblano A, Fernandez-Carrocera LA, Association between intelligence quotient scores and extremely low birth weight in school-age children: Arch Med Res, 2006; 37(5); 639-45

39. Franz AP, Bolat GU, Bolat H, Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and very preterm/very low birth weight: A meta-analysis: Pediatrics, 2018; 141; e20171645

40. Spittle AJ, Treyvaud K, Doyle LW, Early emergence of behavior and social-emotional problems in very preterm infants: J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 2009; 48; 909-18

41. Moore PS, Mokrova I, Frazier JA, Anxiety and depression correlates at age 10 in children born extremely preterm: J Pediatr Psychol, 2021; 46(4); 422-32

42. Marleau I, Vona M, Gagner Ch, Social cognition, adaptive functioning, and behavior problems in preschoolers born extremely preterm: Child Neuropsychol, 2020; 27(1); 96-108

43. Johnson S, Marlow N, Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric disorders: Pediatr Res, 2011; 69(5 pt 2); 11R-18R

44. Indredavik MS, Vik T, Evensen KAI, Perinatal risk and psychiatric outcome in adolescents born preterm with very low birth weight or term small for gestational age: J Dev Behav Pediatr, 2010; 31; 286-94

45. Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, Attention problems in a representative sample of extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children: Dev Neuropsychol, 2011; 36; 57-73

46. Matheis M, Matson JL, Burns CO, Premature birth, low birth weight, and positive screening for autism spectrum disorder in an early intervention sample: J Dev Phys Disabil, 2018; 30; 689-705

47. Treyvaud K, Ure A, Doyle LW, Psychiatric outcomes at age seven for very preterm children: rates and predictors: J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2013; 54; 772-79

48. Jaekel J, Baumann N, Bartmann P, Wolke D, Mood and anxiety disorders in very preterm/very low-birth weight individuals from 6 to 26 years: J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 2017; 59(1); 88-95

49. Witt A, Theurel A, Tolsa CB, Emotional and effortful control abilities in 42-month-old very preterm and full-term children: Early Hum Dev, 2014; 90(10); 565-69

50. Wocadlo C, Rieger I, Social skills and nonverbal decoding of emotions in very preterm children at early school age: Eur J Dev Psychol, 2006; 3(1); 48-70

51. Mossad SI, Muscat C, Pang EW, Taylor M, Emerging atypical connectivity networks for processing angry and fearful faces in very preterm born children: Hum Brain Mapp, 2020; 41(13); 3794-806

52. Jones KM, Champion PR, Woodward LJ, Social competence of preschool children born very preterm: Early Hum Dev, 2013; 89(10); 795-802

53. Boyd LAC, Msall ME, O’Shea TM, Social-emotional delays at 2 years in extremely low gestational age survivors: Correlates of impaired orientation/engagement and emotional regulation: Early Hum Dev, 2013; 89(12); 925-30

54. Potjik MR, de Winter AF, Bos AF, Higher rates of behavioral and emotional problems at preschool age in children born moderately preterm: Arch Dis Child, 2012; 97(2); 112-17

55. Nosarti C, Social relationships, preterm birth or low birth weight, and the brain: JAMA Netw Open, 2019; 2(7); e196960

56. Ritchie K, Bora S, Woodward LJ, Social development of children born very preterm: A systematic review: Dev Med Child Neurol, 2015; 57(10); 899-918

57. Grob A, Meyer CS, Hagmann-von Arx P: Intelligence and development scales, 2009, Bern, Switzerland, Hogrefe

58. Brooks BL, Sherman EM, Strauss E, NEPSY-II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment: Child Neuropsychol, 2009; 16; 80-101

59. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S: NEPSY-II: Clinical and interpretive manual, 2007, San Antonio, TX, The Psychological Corporation

60. Gioia GA, Guy SC, Isquith PK, Kenworthy L, Behavior rating inventory of executive function: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1996

61. Kormos CE, Wilkinson AJ, Davey CJ, Cunningham AJ, Low birth weight and intelligence in adolescence and early adulthood: A meta-analysis: J Public Health (Oxf), 2014; 36(2); 213-24

62. Spittle A, Orton J, Anderson PJ, Early developmental intervention programmes provided post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairment in preterm infants: Cochrane database Syst Rev, 2015; 24(11); CD005495

63. Breeman LD, Jaekel J, Baumann N, Neonatal predictors of cognitive ability in adults born very preterm: A prospective cohort study: Dev Med Child Neurol, 2017; 59; 477-83

64. Stålnacke SR, Tessma M, Böhm B, Herlenius E, Cognitive development trajectories in preterm children with very low birth weight longitudinally followed until 11 years of age: Front Physiol, 2019; 10; 307

65. Beauregard JL, Drews-Botsch C, Sales JM, Does socioeconomic status modify the association between preterm birth and children’s early cognitive ability and kindergarten academic achievement in the United States?: Am J Epidemiol, 2018; 187; 1704-13

66. Manley BJ, Roberts RS, Doyle LW, Social variables predict gains in cognitive scores across the preschool years in children with birth weights 500 to 1250 grams: J Pediatr, 2015; 166; 870-76

Tables

Table 1. Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) comparison of intelligence quotient (IQ) and birth weight.The mean (M) IQ score was 100 with a standard deviation (SD) of 15. ANOVA revealed significant differences (F [1.217]=41.49, P<0.001). Overall, birth weight explained only ~16% of the variance in IQ. The average IQ of children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) was 96.38, while the average IQ of children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) was 12.98 points higher.Table 2. Post hoc power analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ).A post-hoc power analysis revealed a statistical power of 0.99.Table 3. Correlation matrix of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).The results revealed a moderate correlation between the results from the IDS subscales.Table 4. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) and birth weight.Number of observations (N)=219. The Wilks’ lambda value was significant (F [8.210]=7.73, P<0.001) as were all the IDS subscales. The strongest associations (as per the R2 value) were observed for subscales of visual perception, phonological memory, and selective attention. Children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) as a group perform better than children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) on all subscales evaluated.Table 5. Post hoc power analysis of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).A post-hoc power analysis of overall cognitive development revealed a high statistical power (0.99).Table 6. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II) and birth weight.The ANOVA revealed significant differences in socioemotional development (F [1.217]=11.79, P<0.001). Birth weight explained ~4.7% of the total variance in affect recognition.Table 7. Post hoc power analysis of Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II).A post hoc power analysis revealed a high statistical power for this evaluation (0.89).Table 1. Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) comparison of intelligence quotient (IQ) and birth weight.The mean (M) IQ score was 100 with a standard deviation (SD) of 15. ANOVA revealed significant differences (F [1.217]=41.49, P<0.001). Overall, birth weight explained only ~16% of the variance in IQ. The average IQ of children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) was 96.38, while the average IQ of children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) was 12.98 points higher.Table 2. Post hoc power analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ).A post-hoc power analysis revealed a statistical power of 0.99.Table 3. Correlation matrix of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).The results revealed a moderate correlation between the results from the IDS subscales.Table 4. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS) and birth weight.Number of observations (N)=219. The Wilks’ lambda value was significant (F [8.210]=7.73, P<0.001) as were all the IDS subscales. The strongest associations (as per the R2 value) were observed for subscales of visual perception, phonological memory, and selective attention. Children with normal birth weight (NBW ≥2500 g) as a group perform better than children with very low birth weight (VLBW <1500 g) or extremely low birth weight (ELBW <1000 g) on all subscales evaluated.Table 5. Post hoc power analysis of subscales of Intelligence and Development Scales (IDS).A post-hoc power analysis of overall cognitive development revealed a high statistical power (0.99).Table 6. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II) and birth weight.The ANOVA revealed significant differences in socioemotional development (F [1.217]=11.79, P<0.001). Birth weight explained ~4.7% of the total variance in affect recognition.Table 7. Post hoc power analysis of Affect recognition subtest of Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment, second version (NEPSY-II).A post hoc power analysis revealed a high statistical power for this evaluation (0.89).

In Press

18 Mar 2024 : Clinical Research  

Sexual Dysfunction in Women After Tibial Fracture: A Retrospective Comparative Study

Med Sci Monit In Press; DOI: 10.12659/MSM.944136  

0:00

21 Feb 2024 : Clinical Research  

Potential Value of HSP90α in Prognosis of Triple-Negative Breast Cancer

Med Sci Monit In Press; DOI: 10.12659/MSM.943049  

22 Feb 2024 : Review article  

Differentiation of Native Vertebral Osteomyelitis: A Comprehensive Review of Imaging Techniques and Future ...

Med Sci Monit In Press; DOI: 10.12659/MSM.943168  

23 Feb 2024 : Clinical Research  

A Study of 60 Patients with Low Back Pain to Compare Outcomes Following Magnetotherapy, Ultrasound, Laser, ...

Med Sci Monit In Press; DOI: 10.12659/MSM.943732  

Most Viewed Current Articles

16 May 2023 : Clinical Research  

Electrophysiological Testing for an Auditory Processing Disorder and Reading Performance in 54 School Stude...

DOI :10.12659/MSM.940387

Med Sci Monit 2023; 29:e940387

0:00

17 Jan 2024 : Review article  

Vaccination Guidelines for Pregnant Women: Addressing COVID-19 and the Omicron Variant

DOI :10.12659/MSM.942799

Med Sci Monit 2024; 30:e942799

0:00

14 Dec 2022 : Clinical Research  

Prevalence and Variability of Allergen-Specific Immunoglobulin E in Patients with Elevated Tryptase Levels

DOI :10.12659/MSM.937990

Med Sci Monit 2022; 28:e937990

0:00

01 Jan 2022 : Editorial  

Editorial: Current Status of Oral Antiviral Drug Treatments for SARS-CoV-2 Infection in Non-Hospitalized Pa...

DOI :10.12659/MSM.935952

Med Sci Monit 2022; 28:e935952

0:00

Your Privacy

We use cookies to ensure the functionality of our website, to personalize content and advertising, to provide social media features, and to analyze our traffic. If you allow us to do so, we also inform our social media, advertising and analysis partners about your use of our website, You can decise for yourself which categories you you want to deny or allow. Please note that based on your settings not all functionalities of the site are available. View our privacy policy.

Medical Science Monitor eISSN: 1643-3750
Medical Science Monitor eISSN: 1643-3750